Overhearing a language during childhood pdf




















Franken, T. Brief report: Are children with autism proficient word learners? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40 2 , — Gampe, A. Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language, 32 3 , — Hani, H. Journal of Child Language, 40 5 , — Hart, B. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children Vol. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing. Hedge, C. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences.

Behavior Research Methods. Jaswal, V. Learning proper and common names in inferential versus ostensive contexts. Child Development, 72 3 , — Korhonen, V. Autism spectrum disorder and impaired joint attention: A review of joint attention research from the past decade. Nordic Psychology, 66 2 , 94— Lord, C. Torrance: Western Psychological Services. Luyster, R. Word learning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45 6 , — Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38 8 , — Magiati, I. Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood.

Clinical Psychology Review, 34 1 , 73— Mawhood, L. Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Naigles, L. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 27 , 11— Waterloo, ON: Knowledge in Development.

The language use inventory for young children: A parent-report measure of pragmatic language development for to month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50 1 , — Ochs, E. Language acquisition and socializations: Three developmental stories and their implications.

LeVine Eds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Oshima-Takane, Y. Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 15 01 , 95— An alternative view of pronominal errors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19 1 , 73— Birth order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67 2 , — Parish-Morris, J. Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning.

Child Development, 78 4 , — Pepperberg, I. Proposed use of two-part interactive modeling as a means to increase functional skills in children with a variety of disabilities. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 12 4 , — Use of two-trainer interactive modeling as a potential means to engender social behavior in children with various disabilities. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 15 2 , — Pierce, K. Social perception in children with autism: An attentional deficit?

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27 3 , — Sandbank, M. Language policy and language ideologies in Szekler Land Rumania : A promotion of bilingualism? Knightly, L. Production benefits of childhood overhearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1 , — Maguire, M.

Cohen, K. McAlister, K. MacSwan Eds. Somerville, Cascadilla Press. Cognate recognition and L3 vocabulary acquisition. Acta Universitatis Sapientiae, Philologica, 2 2 , — Oh, J. Holding on to childhood language memory. Cognition, 86 3 , B53—B Oller, D. All-day recordings to investigate vocabulary development: A case study of a trilingual toddler. National Library of Medicine. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31 4 , — Rothman, J. L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The typological primacy model.

Second Language Research, 27 1 , — Between support and marginalisation: The development of academic language in linguistic minority children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 7 2—3 , — Wang, X. Growing up with three languages. Yaghejian, A. Multiliteracy in three linguistically and culturally diverse heritage language schools. Colloquium presentation national council of organizations of less commonly taught languages.

Fourth annual conference Arlington, VA. Iulia Pittman 1 Email author 1. Personalised recommendations. Cite chapter How to cite? ENW EndNote. In particular, Oshima-Takane found that laterborn siblings have an advantage over firstborns in learning personal pronouns, presumably because they have more opportunities to overhear pronouns being used in conversations between their siblings and parents. The present studies extend this research in at least two ways: 1 by demonstrating that word learning through overhearing can also be achieved in a single learning episode and 2 by demonstrating that young children can learn other word types object labels, action verbs through overhearing.

Of course, the experimental sit- uation we employed is likely much simpler than real-life overhearing contexts in that the children were seated in view of the adults and there was little to distract them from attending to the interaction between those adults.

Although studies that systematically vary the level of surrounding, and potentially distracting, activity need to be conducted, informal conversations with the parents of our participants suggest that children may indeed be capable of learning vocabulary in these more challenging settings. Many parents reported that their children knew many more words than they had been explicitly taught including some words that parents would prefer their children had not learned! Several studies have demonstrated that the amount of time young children spend in face-to-face conversations varies significantly across cultural groups.

In summary, the present studies provide direct research evidence in support of the idea that children acquire vocabulary in nondidactic, nonostensive contexts.

Specifically, the findings support claims that young children are quite adept at monitoring third-party conversations, and provide experimental evidence that they can acquire vocabulary in such situations. Together with the results of ethnographic studies demonstrating very little direct language teaching in interactions between adults and young children, these results highlight the active role played by children in acquiring language, and point to the possibility that didactic interactions might be the exception rather than the rule in the contexts in which young children in all cultures acquire their early vocabulary.

Special thanks go to Margarita Azmitia, Lois Bloom, and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

Jennifer Jipson and Maureen A. Callanan are also at the University of California at Santa Cruz. Twenty-four-month-old children learn words for absent objects and actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 79— Akhtar, N.

Intersubjectivity in early language learning and use. Braten Ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. The social nature of words and word learning. Hirsh-Pasek Eds. Oxford, U. Baldwin, D. Child Development, 62, — Journal of Child Language, 20, — Understanding the link between joint attention and language. Dunham Eds. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Child Development, 67, — Barton, M. Joint attention and conversation in mother-infant-sibling triads. Bloom, L. Language acquisition in its developmental context.

Siegler Eds. Damon Series Ed. Cognition, perception, and language pp. New York: Wiley. The intentionality model of word learning: How to learn a word, any word. Brown, P. Conversational structure and language acquisition: The role of repetition in Tzeltal adult and child speech.

Journal of Linguistic Anthropology. Callanan, M. Learning words in directive and ostensive contexts. Manuscript submitted for publication. Choi, S.

Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 22, — Crago, M. Exploring innateness through cultural and linguistic variation.

Gopnik Ed. New York: Oxford University Press. Dunham, P. Joint-attentional states and lexical acquisition at 18 months. Developmental Psychology, 29, — Dunn, J. Becoming a conversationalist despite or because of having an older sibling. Child Development, 60, — Fernald, A.

Child Development, 64, — Forrester, M. Affording social-cognitive skills in young children: The overhearing context. Turner Eds. New York: St. Fox Tree, J. Listening in on monologues and dialogues. Discourse Processes, 27, 35— Gentner, D. On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb meaning.

Child Development, 49, — Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. Kuczaj Ed. Language, thought, and culture pp. Goodman, J. The role of semantic context and memory in the acquisition of novel nouns. Child Development, 69, — Gopnik, A. Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs.

Merriman Eds. Lieven, E. Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. Richards Eds. Cambridge, U. Messer, D. The identification of names in maternal speech to infants.

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 69— Nelson, K. Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Oshima-Takane, Y. Children learn from speech not addressed to them: The case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 15, 95— The learning of first- and second-person pronouns in English. Jackendoff, P. Wynn Eds. Birth-order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Reznick, J. A multiple form word production checklist for assessing early language.

Journal of Child Language, 16, 91— Rogoff, B. Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Saffran, J. Incidental language learning: Listening and learning out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, — Schieffelin, B. The give and take of everyday life: Language socialization of Kaluli children. A cultural perspective on the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. Golinkoff Ed.

Shady, M. Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 26, — Tardif, T. Developmental Psychology, 32, — Journal of Child Language, 24, — Tomasello, M. Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, — Joint attention and early language.

Child Development, 57, — Acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 19, — Zukow-Goldring, P.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000